The Riddle of the Human Species
The
Stone is a forum for contemporary philosophers on issues both
timely and timeless.
The task of understanding humanity
is too important and too daunting to leave to the humanities. Their many
branches, from philosophy to law to history and the creative arts, have
described the particularities of human nature with genius and exquisite detail,
back and forth in endless permutations. But they have not explained why we
possess our special nature and not some other out of a vast number of
conceivable possibilities. In that sense, the humanities have not accounted for
a full understanding of our species’ existence.
So, just what are we? The key to
the great riddle lies in the circumstance and process that created our species.
The human condition is a product of history, not just the six millenniums of
civilization but very much further back, across hundreds of millenniums. The
whole of it, biological and cultural evolution, in seamless unity, must be
explored for an answer to the mystery. When thus viewed across its entire
traverse, the history of humanity also becomes the key to learning how and why
our species survived.
A majority of people prefer to
interpret history as the unfolding of a supernatural design, to whose author we
owe obedience. But that comforting interpretation has grown less supportable as
knowledge of the real world has expanded. Scientific knowledge (measured by
numbers of scientists and scientific journals) in particular has been doubling
every 10 to 20 years for over a century. In traditional explanations of the
past, religious creation stories have been blended with the humanities to
attribute meaning to our species’s existence. It is time to consider what
science might give to the humanities and the humanities to science in a common
search for a more solidly grounded answer to the great riddle.
To begin, biologists have found
that the biological origin of advanced social behavior in humans was similar to
that occurring elsewhere in the animal kingdom. Using comparative studies of
thousands of animal species, from insects to mammals, they have concluded that
the most complex societies have arisen through eusociality — roughly, “true”
social condition. The members of a eusocial group cooperatively rear the young
across multiple generations. They also divide labor through the surrender by
some members of at least some of their personal reproduction in a way that
increases the “reproductive success” (lifetime reproduction) of other members.
Eusociality stands out as an oddity
in a couple of ways. One is its extreme rarity. Out of hundreds of thousands of
evolving lines of animals on the land during the past 400 million years, the
condition, so far as we can determine, has arisen only about two dozen times.
This is likely to be an underestimate, due to sampling error. Nevertheless, we
can be certain that the number of originations was very small.
Furthermore, the known eusocial
species arose very late in the history of life. It appears to have occurred not
at all during the great Paleozoic diversification of insects, 350 to 250
million years before the present, during which the variety of insects
approached that of today. Nor is there as yet any evidence of eusocial species
during the Mesozoic Era until the appearance of the earliest termites and ants
between 200 and 150 million years ago. Humans at the Homo level appeared only
very recently, following tens of millions of years of evolution among the
primates.
Once attained, advanced social
behavior at the eusocial grade has proved a major ecological success. Of the
two dozen independent lines, just two within the insects — ants and termites —
globally dominate invertebrates on the land. Although they are represented by
fewer than 20 thousand of the million known living insect species, ants and
termites compose more than half of the world’s insect body weight.
The history of eusociality raises a
question: given the enormous advantage it confers, why was this advanced form
of social behavior so rare and long delayed? The answer appears to be the
special sequence of preliminary evolutionary changes that must occur before the
final step to eusociality can be taken. In all of the eusocial species analyzed
to date, the final step before eusociality is the construction of a protected
nest, from which foraging trips begin and within which the young are raised to
maturity. The original nest builders can be a lone female, a mated pair, or a
small and weakly organized group. When this final preliminary step is attained,
all that is needed to create a eusocial colony is for the parents and offspring
to stay at the nest and cooperate in raising additional generations of young.
Such primitive assemblages then divide easily into risk-prone foragers and
risk-averse parents and nurses.
What brought one primate line to
the rare level of eusociality? Paleontologists have found that the
circumstances were humble. In Africa about two million years ago, one species
of the primarily vegetarian australopithecine evidently shifted its diet to include
a much higher reliance on meat. For a group to harvest such a high-energy,
widely dispersed source of food, it did not pay to roam about as a loosely
organized pack of adults and young like present-day chimpanzees and bonobos. It
was more efficient to occupy a campsite (thus, the nest) and send out hunters
who could bring home meat, either killed or scavenged, to share with others. In
exchange, the hunters received protection of the campsite and their own young
offspring kept there.
From studies of modern humans,
including hunter-gatherers, whose lives tell us so much about human origins,
social psychologists have deduced the mental growth that began with hunting and
campsites. A premium was placed on personal relationships geared to both
competition and cooperation among the members. The process was ceaselessly
dynamic and demanding. It far exceeded in intensity anything similar
experienced by the roaming, loosely organized bands of most animal societies.
It required a memory good enough to assess the intentions of fellow members, to
predict their responses, from one moment to the next; and it resulted in the
ability to invent and inwardly rehearse competing scenarios of future
interactions.
The social intelligence of the
campsite-anchored prehumans evolved as a kind of non-stop game of chess. Today,
at the terminus of this evolutionary process, our immense memory banks are
smoothly activated across the past, present, and future. They allow us to
evaluate the prospects and consequences variously of alliances, bonding, sexual
contact, rivalries, domination, deception, loyalty and betrayal. We
instinctively delight in the telling of countless stories about others as
players upon the inner stage. The best of it is expressed in the creative arts,
political theory, and other higher-level activities we have come to call the
humanities.
The definitive part of the long
creation story evidently began with the primitive Homo habilis (or a species
closely related to it) two million years ago. Prior to the habilines the
prehumans had been animals. Largely vegetarians, they had human-like bodies,
but their cranial capacity remained chimpanzee-size, at or below 500 cubic
centimeters. Starting with the habiline period the capacity grew precipitously:
to 680 cubic centimeters in Homo habilis, 900 in Homo erectus, and about 1,400
in Homo sapiens. The expansion of the human brain was one of the most rapid
episodes of evolution of complex organs in the history of life.
Still, to recognize the rare coming
together of cooperating primates is not enough to account for the full
potential of modern humans that brain capacity provides. Evolutionary
biologists have searched for the grandmaster of advanced social evolution, the
combination of forces and environmental circumstances that bestowed greater
longevity and more successful reproduction on the possession of high social
intelligence. At present there are two competing theories of the principal
force. The first is kin selection: individuals favor collateral kin (relatives
other than offspring) making it easier for altruism to evolve among members of
the same group. Altruism in turn engenders complex social organization, and, in
the one case that involves big mammals, human-level intelligence.
The second, more recently argued
theory (full disclosure: I am one of the modern version’s authors), the
grandmaster is multilevel selection. This formulation recognizes two levels at
which natural selection operates: individual selection based on competition and
cooperation among members of the same group, and group selection, which arises
from competition and cooperation between groups. Multilevel selection is
gaining in favor among evolutionary biologists because of a recent mathematical
proof that kin selection can arise only under special conditions that
demonstrably do not exist, and the better fit of multilevel selection to all of
the two dozen known animal cases of eusocial evolution.
The roles of both individual and
group selection are indelibly stamped (to borrow a phrase from Charles Darwin)
upon our social behavior. As expected, we are intensely interested in the
minutiae of behavior of those around us. Gossip is a prevailing subject of
conversation, everywhere from hunter-gatherer campsites to royal courts. The
mind is a kaleidoscopically shifting map of others, each of whom is drawn
emotionally in shades of trust, love, hatred, suspicion, admiration, envy and
sociability. We are compulsively driven to create and belong to groups,
variously nested, overlapping or separate, and large or small. Almost all
groups compete with those of similar kind in some manner or other. We tend to
think of our own as superior, and we find our identity within them.
The existence of competition and
conflict, the latter often violent, has been a hallmark of societies as far
back as archaeological evidence is able to offer. These and other traits we
call human nature are so deeply resident in our emotions and habits of thought
as to seem just part of some greater nature, like the air we all breathe, and
the molecular machinery that drives all of life. But they are not. Instead,
they are among the idiosyncratic hereditary traits that define our species.
The major features of the
biological origins of our species are coming into focus, and with this
clarification the potential of a more fruitful contact between science and the
humanities. The convergence between these two great branches of learning will
matter hugely when enough people have thought it through. On the science side,
genetics, the brain sciences, evolutionary biology, and paleontology will be
seen in a different light. Students will be taught prehistory as well as
conventional history, the whole presented as the living world’s greatest epic.
We will also, I believe, take a more
serious look at our place in nature. Exalted we are indeed, risen to be the
mind of the biosphere without a doubt, our spirits capable of awe and ever more
breathtaking leaps of imagination. But we are still part of earth’s fauna and
flora. We are bound to it by emotion, physiology, and not least, deep history.
It is dangerous to think of this planet as a way station to a better world, or
continue to convert it into a literal, human-engineered spaceship. Contrary to
general opinion, demons and gods do not vie for our allegiance. We are
self-made, independent, alone and fragile. Self-understanding is what counts
for long-term survival, both for individuals and for the species.
Edward O. Wilson is Honorary
Curator in Entomology and University Research Professor Emeritus, Harvard
University. He has received more than 100 awards for his research and writing,
including the U. S. National Medal of Science, the Crafoord Prize and two
Pulitzer Prizes in non-fiction. His most recent book is “The Social Conquest of
Earth.”
No comments:
Post a Comment